Psycholinguistics: Crash Course Linguistics #11 - By CrashCourse
Transcript
00:0-1 | Hi , I'm taylor and welcome to crash course linguistics | |
00:02 | . Language happens thanks to the brain . This spongy | |
00:05 | thing sitting in our skulls is responsible for our abilities | |
00:08 | to acquire complex linguistic skills like phrenology , semantics , | |
00:11 | syntax and reading . It's what helps us great poems | |
00:14 | and composed texts or get the meaning of a raised | |
00:17 | eyebrow . Our string of random emojis understanding the brain | |
00:20 | helps us understand how languages produced and interpreted . We | |
00:24 | can point directly to the parts of our mouths and | |
00:26 | hands that are used to make language but you can't | |
00:29 | reach in and feel your own brain . Luckily over | |
00:31 | the years , people have devised a whole range of | |
00:34 | ways of looking at the brain to figure out where | |
00:36 | and how language happens . This is the field of | |
00:40 | psycho linguistics . Mm Interest in the relationship between language | |
00:54 | and the brain really picked up in the 19th century | |
00:57 | . Researchers observed people with language disorders and then looked | |
01:00 | at their brains after they died . If damage to | |
01:02 | one part of the brain from a stroke , dementia | |
01:05 | or a head injury correlated with a difference in the | |
01:07 | subject's ability to understand or produce language , the scientists | |
01:11 | could predict that there was a relationship there . These | |
01:13 | kinds of injuries that affect our language abilities are known | |
01:16 | as F . Asia , to famous kinds of aphasia | |
01:18 | discovered at the time are called Broca's aphasia , and | |
01:21 | veronica's aphasia , the areas of the brain related to | |
01:24 | each kind of aphasia therefore became known as Broca's area | |
01:28 | and veronica's area . You may have heard about them | |
01:30 | in a psychology or anatomy class , Broca's area is | |
01:33 | located around your left temple and it was named after | |
01:36 | Paul Broca , 1/19 century physician who noticed that people | |
01:40 | who had been injured in this part of their brain | |
01:42 | acted in similar ways well , they could still understand | |
01:45 | language . They could only produce maybe a handful of | |
01:48 | words one at a time , Broca's area affected their | |
01:51 | ability to speak or sign in a fluid grammatical way | |
01:55 | . In other words , it affected their ability to | |
01:57 | use more fa syntax . Right around the time Broca | |
02:00 | was making his discovery , german physician Carl veronica discovered | |
02:04 | that if a different part of the brain was injured | |
02:06 | , there was a totally different effects . People injured | |
02:09 | in this spot located just above your left ear , | |
02:12 | tended to talk in a way that sounded fluent and | |
02:14 | grammatical but was nonsensical verticals area is associated with the | |
02:19 | meaning of language , but those 19th century studies were | |
02:22 | limited and the brain is amazingly complex and flexible . | |
02:26 | More recent research has found that some people can majorly | |
02:29 | damaged Broca's area and never developed aphasia . Other people | |
02:33 | can relearn how to speak through extensive practice , building | |
02:36 | on their ability to sing , which is controlled by | |
02:39 | a different part of the brain . These newer studies | |
02:41 | help us understand neural plasticity , the ability of the | |
02:45 | brain to flexibly build and connect parts of the brain | |
02:48 | in response to injury or as part of learning . | |
02:50 | And though the language areas are usually located on the | |
02:52 | left hemisphere of the brain , some people's language areas | |
02:55 | are found predominantly in the right hemisphere or spread across | |
02:58 | both sides , especially for left handed or ambidextrous people | |
03:01 | . So the relationship between language and the brain is | |
03:04 | even more complicated than we first thought . Even now | |
03:06 | , errors and differences and language use can teach us | |
03:09 | about the different skills involved in language and how they're | |
03:12 | organized inside our minds . We all sometimes forget a | |
03:15 | word that we know perfectly well or accidentally swap words | |
03:18 | , parts of words or idioms what you might encounter | |
03:21 | a spoon , Tourism's tip of the tongue experiences or | |
03:23 | mixed metaphors . These production errors tell us valuable things | |
03:27 | about how the mind handles language like you know , | |
03:29 | when you just can't quite remember a word , you | |
03:32 | know it , you almost have it , it's right | |
03:34 | there , you just can't retrieve it . This phenomenon | |
03:38 | is known as a tip of the tongue experience . | |
03:40 | And psycho linguists have found that people with a word | |
03:43 | on the tips of their tongues can often recall other | |
03:45 | information about it . They might remember its meaning , | |
03:48 | its first letter and sometimes how many syllables it has | |
03:51 | , but they can't quite recall the complete word signed | |
03:54 | languages also have this phenomenon which is known as the | |
03:56 | tip of the fingers naturally . And signers experiencing tip | |
04:00 | of the fingers can also recall certain information about the | |
04:03 | sign they're seeking , especially the initial hand shape and | |
04:06 | location of the hand . They just can't recall a | |
04:08 | movement . They need to complete the sign tip of | |
04:10 | the tongue . And finger experiences can show us how | |
04:13 | our thoughts are organized because we can have access to | |
04:15 | the first letter or initial hand position without having access | |
04:19 | to the remaining sounds or movement . Knowing a word | |
04:21 | isn't a binary state of yes or no . Like | |
04:24 | a computer . Our brains can also retain partial information | |
04:28 | . Production errors are so useful that psycho linguists have | |
04:31 | techniques for trying to get people to make even more | |
04:33 | of them so that they can study those errors . | |
04:35 | In a laboratory setting , psycho linguists can induce tip | |
04:38 | of the tongue or finger experiences by asking people to | |
04:42 | translate words or recall proper . Now , let's head | |
04:44 | to the thought bubble to try another psycho linguistic experiment | |
04:47 | . Right here in a moment to shapes are going | |
04:50 | to appear on the screen . Let's decide which one | |
04:53 | is called kiki and which one is called bouba . | |
04:56 | Are you ready ? It's more than likely you called | |
05:00 | the shape on the left cuba and the shape on | |
05:02 | the right kiki . About nine out of every 10 | |
05:04 | people make that choice . Experiments have repeatedly shown that | |
05:07 | we think of voiceless stops like and high vowels E | |
05:12 | as small , sharp , crunchy , bright and spiky | |
05:16 | compared to voice sounds like buddha and rounded vowels like | |
05:19 | Ooh , which are big , lumpy , dark and | |
05:22 | heavy across many different languages . The kiki buba experiment | |
05:26 | shows us that language is related to other sensory experiences | |
05:29 | . Yes . Words are still made up of arbitrary | |
05:32 | smaller parts but it's not completely 100% arbitrary . And | |
05:36 | this mapping between senses can show up in some general | |
05:39 | tendencies for naming things across language . One large study | |
05:42 | showed that words for knows were more likely to have | |
05:45 | a nasal sound like mama orna . Across many different | |
05:48 | languages , marketers are very aware of these links . | |
05:51 | Buba wouldn't make a good name for a brand of | |
05:53 | crackers , but kiki would because we generally want our | |
05:57 | crackers to be crispy . But I'm sure Buba brand | |
05:59 | ice cream would taste much better round and smooth and | |
06:02 | creamy despite these general tendencies . There are also language | |
06:06 | specific differences . If your language doesn't have a book | |
06:09 | or you sound , you might not think of buba | |
06:12 | as a possible word so you might not associated consistently | |
06:15 | with the blobby shape . Different languages can also label | |
06:18 | the shapes differently depending on how their sound systems work | |
06:21 | . Town can influence how mandarin speakers label these shapes | |
06:23 | . The human brain doesn't completely separate our linguistic knowledge | |
06:26 | from other knowledge of the world . And experiments like | |
06:29 | the kiki bouba . Test . Help show that . | |
06:31 | Thanks . Thought bubble or should I say thought buba | |
06:34 | . That's one kind of psycho linguistic experiment . But | |
06:36 | it's far from the only one psycho linguists might use | |
06:39 | a prime ng experiment to test how closely words are | |
06:42 | related in the brain . They prime the participant with | |
06:45 | one word and measure the speed of their responses to | |
06:47 | other words . Say a subject is primed with the | |
06:50 | word dog and then has a faster response to cat | |
06:52 | than to other words . We might conclude cat and | |
06:55 | dog are more closely related in the brain . We | |
06:57 | can also use gating experiments where we measure how much | |
07:00 | of a word a participant needs to hear or see | |
07:03 | until they know I'm saying park instead of parked gating | |
07:06 | experiments show that sounds aren't always produced in discrete sequences | |
07:10 | , like our alphabet , makes them look like most | |
07:12 | english speakers will produce the co sound in cuba a | |
07:15 | little bit differently than the coast sound . In calm | |
07:18 | psycho linguists have even looked into such mysteries as weather | |
07:21 | , swearing helps us manage pain in that study psycho | |
07:24 | linguists compared how long people could hold their hand in | |
07:27 | a bucket of ice water when they were allowed to | |
07:29 | swear and when they were not , when people were | |
07:32 | allowed to swear , they could hold their hand in | |
07:34 | the ice water for longer . Mm I'm definitely going | |
07:37 | to find a practical application for this . Other ways | |
07:40 | of figuring out what's going on in the brain when | |
07:41 | we use language , involve using various kinds of equipment | |
07:44 | . Eye tracking studies , try to figure out what | |
07:46 | we're thinking about based on what we're looking at . | |
07:48 | Let's say we're reading a sentence like this one . | |
07:51 | Now the rabbit crouched on the cushion is a totally | |
07:54 | reasonable english sentence . So that's where most people assume | |
07:57 | it's going at first . But then when we get | |
07:59 | to the words seemed we need to reevaluate . That's | |
08:02 | where I tracking shows that a lot of people look | |
08:04 | back at the earlier portion of the sentence to figure | |
08:06 | out what's going on . In this case , a | |
08:08 | structure more like the rabbit that was crouched on the | |
08:11 | cushion , seemed friendly , misleading sentences like these are | |
08:15 | called garden path sentences because they seem to lead you | |
08:18 | up the garden path of one interpretation before you realize | |
08:21 | there's actually a different structure going on . Eye tracking | |
08:24 | and garden path sentences show us that we process sentences | |
08:27 | as we're experiencing them . We don't wait until we've | |
08:30 | seen or heard a whole sentence before starting to figure | |
08:32 | out what's going on , electroencephalogram fee or E . | |
08:36 | G . Records the electrical activity of neurons firing through | |
08:39 | a cap of small sensors on the scalp . A | |
08:41 | psycho linguists might hook a person out to an E | |
08:43 | . G . And say a sentence like my favorite | |
08:46 | ice cream is chocolate and socks , socks is semantically | |
08:49 | unexpected in a sentence that we assumed would be about | |
08:52 | food so the brain reacts accordingly . And an E | |
08:55 | E . G . Is especially good at indicating when | |
08:57 | a surgeon electricity happens . So here it might map | |
09:00 | a kind of surge known as an N 400 around | |
09:03 | 400 milliseconds . After hearing socks , eggs are quiet | |
09:07 | and relatively affordable , but they can be disturbed even | |
09:10 | by small things like blinking . Plus they're not that | |
09:14 | great at mapping where things happen in the brain , | |
09:16 | functional magnetic resonance imaging or FmRI , on the other | |
09:19 | hand , is relatively precise in locating brain activity getting | |
09:23 | within a few millimeters of where the activity is happening | |
09:26 | . It does this by measuring when there's increased oxygen | |
09:29 | in parts of the brain . The more neural activity | |
09:31 | you're thinking , the more blood goes to the area | |
09:33 | of the brain and that blood brings a lot of | |
09:36 | oxygen to help those busy neurons . For example a | |
09:38 | psycho linguists might have someone learn and recite back a | |
09:41 | few words in a made up language to see what | |
09:44 | happens in the brain . When we try to learn | |
09:45 | a new language . While Fmri is relatively precise in | |
09:48 | locating brain activity , it's less precise at when that | |
09:52 | activity is happening , it only gets within a few | |
09:54 | seconds while thoughts can happen in fractions of a second | |
09:58 | . They're also very expensive and pretty dang uncomfortable to | |
10:01 | hang out in . So there's sort of a trade | |
10:03 | off . E . G machines are precise about time | |
10:05 | but imprecise about space whereas MRI machines are precise about | |
10:09 | space but imprecise about time . These machines with their | |
10:12 | precise data and complex graphs might seem like just the | |
10:15 | thing 19th century researchers like broken and veronica needed to | |
10:19 | understand the link between the brain and language but really | |
10:22 | we need to approach them with just as much caution | |
10:24 | as those older experiments . There's still a lot of | |
10:27 | individual variation and how our brains get organized as we | |
10:30 | learn things and lots of psycho linguistics work has been | |
10:34 | done with small numbers of people who speak dominant languages | |
10:37 | like english . So we only know a little about | |
10:39 | if and how being fluent in other languages affects what | |
10:43 | happens in the brain . There's always more to learn | |
10:46 | . See you next time when we talk about how | |
10:47 | we learn language in the first place . Thanks for | |
10:49 | watching this episode of crush chris linguistics . If you | |
10:52 | want to help keep all crash course free for everybody | |
10:55 | forever , you can join our community on Patreon . |
Summarizer
DESCRIPTION:
OVERVIEW:
Psycholinguistics: Crash Course Linguistics #11 is a free educational video by CrashCourse.
This page not only allows students and teachers view Psycholinguistics: Crash Course Linguistics #11 videos but also find engaging Sample Questions, Apps, Pins, Worksheets, Books related to the following topics.